Every year there is at least one lecture I eagerly
await and look forward to, namely the Quinn Memorial Lecture Series. It has been my
fifth attendance over the last six years, with my first one being Michael Gazzaniga’s exquisite lecture entitled Free Will and the Science of the Brain. Unfortunately, I had to miss last year’s lecture
by Dr Robert T. Knight because the date and time interfered with my work schedule,
but I had been luckier this time around.
Although it certainly did not look like it at first. The
scheduled talk Learning
and Attention in Infants: The Importance of Prediction in Development by Distinguished Scientist Richard Aslin was suddenly
canceled and postponed to a later indefinite date. No reason nor future date
were given at the time. Could it be that my brain would have to go two years in
a row without the much needed and much desired annual adrenaline shot of
knowledge?
Thank goodness, my worries were unfounded, and there I was seated upfront with my smartphone in hand to take
notes in preparation for this blog post. I did not know what to expect and on paper, at least for
me, the topic at hand about how infants learn language and make predictions
about their surroundings did not have the similar emotional impact on me as did
previous topics and titles of this wonderful series.
There was certainly nothing wrong with the subject, but for
me personally, it came about a decade too late as my son has already taken his
first steps into the preteen period. More importantly, I did not wish to hear
in retrospect how I may have potentially failed him as a parent in terms of
language learning and / or behavior.
Yet again for a second time, my worries were entirely unfounded.
The talk itself was much more interesting and engaging than I had imagined. First,
Dr. Aslin told us how infants learn through auditory statistical learning. This
means that they break streams of words, which are for them initially nothing
but random sounds, into auditory chunks, hence creating word boundaries.
These boundaries are often signaled in fluent speech with
pauses, such as taking breath. These word chunks then are basically processed and
analyzed by the infant brain to make predictions. In order to be able to better
understand and predict their surroundings, babies have the tendency to listen
and pay more attention to novel and infrequent words and tones.
Why? Because by knowing and establishing certain patterns,
they can better understand the rules. This is not a case of merely memorizing
words but also looking past them for meaning (vocabulary as a symbol of a
designated thing / event in the world) and learning about the inherent rules
(grammar, sentence structure and appropriate word choices).
In a way then, it
is memorization plus generalization, respectively known as model-free and
model-based learning; the latter of which is generally designated as smart, abstract
and flexible ways of comprehension, while the former is rather unfairly treated as the
opposite of all those epithets.
Certainly, there is also incidental learning. This means
that we absorb knowledge and information without particularly focusing on the
given stimuli. This type of learning would occur when one is performing a task by also taking in background noise or information in an almost automatic or
subconscious manner.
Evidently, paying attention is much better suited for
learning, but even when we do not notice stimuli implicitly, we are still aware
of and capable of remembering strands of information around us without having
to specifically focus on them.
The manner they tested all of this was very interesting. The
researchers inundated infants with random sounds and stimuli, both in terms of
nonsense words, i.e. random syllable sounds as well as tones. Babies tended to
be interested in new stimuli, but whenever they managed to discern a clear and
repeated pattern, they would lose interest.
This occurred because infants were able to predict the next
sound, so the sequence did not provide any novel information for them. Once a
pattern was established, the baby moved on to something else … unless there was
an unexpected result. That is, if they were expecting a given sound to follow,
but either it did not, or the pattern was changed, then the element of surprise
would warrant and elicit their attention again.
This happens mainly due to the structure of our brain. To
clarify this tendency of the brain to create, establish and predict patterns,
they conducted an interesting experiment with pairs of tones. The researchers
would play the sound of two honking horns beep
beep. After repeated exposure, the baby expected them to come in pairs, yet
when the researchers omitted the second beep,
the brain nonetheless supplied it.
This was discovered by hooking up wires on babies (no
worries, this is harmless and painless standard procedure), thereby noting the infant’s
brain activity. In other words, when the brain registered the first honk, the
second one was immediately supplied by the brain, regardless if it was or was not there!
Since our brains are wired to make sense of our environment
in terms of words or tones, we would use top-down processing once a pattern has
been established, meaning that the higher structures of the brain would override
the lower ones. In the previous experiment, the higher brain regions literally expected the double
tone.
Once inferences were made, babies would then allocate
attention to new information. This was observed by their behavior and
reactions, such as looking longer at unexpected stimuli or looking away from
expected, hence “boring” and unstimulating stimuli.
In that sense, the brain
works tandem with behavior, we are able to see connections and patterns and
then start looking for them, hence it is the brain structure that is grounded
and established first before the behavior sets in and manifests itself.
About 9 months of age, infants start searching for hidden
objects because their brain - and with it their imagination - has developed to
a state where the infants are capable of doing and perceiving such a thing; by
around 18 months, they can produce two-word sentences. This seems to be
universal and is caused by brain development growth and changes.
Yet some of the startling, if not downright shocking,
finding was in relation to premature babies. It turns out that they can be at a
significant disadvantage when it comes to their brain development although this
may not be immediately visible or discernible in their behavior.
Put differently, premature babies may act and behave the
same way as other infants, but their brain is less developed than their
full-term counterparts. That sent shock-waves down my spine as my son was born
prematurely! However, if you have premature children, or if by chance, you are
one yourself, keep in mind that this is not always the case.
First off, each case or person is different, and it is not
necessarily a disadvantage to begin with. In fact, the premature baby may make
up for brain development at a later stage. Due to the plasticity of this magnificent
and complex organ of ours, the brain can compensate for parts that have not
fully developed, even more so at a younger and developing age. We should also
note that the studies were conducted with babies that were significantly
premature by about a handful of months and not by a mere month as it was in the
case of my son.
Moreover, there are two other general factors that are significant
and essential for learning as well. One of them is the fact that salience and
prior knowledge tend to drive attention. Anything that is surprising and simply,
or maybe ostentatiously, stands out will draw the attention of infants, an
observation that is probably equally true for the adult age. The commonplace,
however, is generally not worth a second look.
The other factor of importance is what is generally known
and referred to as the Goldilocks effect. This simply means that if the
information or stimuli presented is too simple or too complex for the baby, he
or she will simply look away and lose interest.
The Goldilocks effect plays
also a significant role when it comes to stress and anxiety of children at
school; optimal attention and learning is usually achieved when the material
and / or environment is neither too comfortable and relaxing nor too stressful
and exacting. The middle ground, i.e. the Goldilocks effect, is usually optimal
for learning.
But another question that arose was why was it that we as
adults lose that infant ability to make sense of our environment? For instance,
this type of processing information would be most useful when learning a second
or additional language. Why was the same process not supplied to us at a later
stage since it would make our language learning – and life - so much easier?
Part of the problem stems from the fact that as adults we
have already established preconceived and set ways of learning and of reacting
to our environment, commonly known as entrenched learning. Since we can make
more and better sense of our surrounding, and we already have a plethora of prior
information and knowledge to select from, we can predict it much better. As a result, and for
better or worse, we are not so much drawn to new stimuli and information, but, in a
sense, we lose some of our capacity for curiosity and wonder.
But this is perhaps not the only reason we become somewhat jaded
as adults. The other driving force, an issue that came up during my personal
conversation with Dr. Aslin afterwards, was anxiety. When we are young, we are generally driven by our anxiety to make sense of everything that is around us, as it
could spell potential threat and danger to our health and wellbeing. Once we
have sorted out the information, we somewhat lose or at least soften that
anxious edge.
With less anxiety, there is also less need to fear or worry
about new stimuli. Dr. Aslin called this the dichotomy of an exploring baby versus an exploiting adult brain. While as
infants, we strive to look for clues and knowledge to make predictions, as
somewhat “jaded” adults, we want to use whatever new knowledge we get our hands
on to better serve our benefit and purposes. Most of these developments may
origin in the brain and are hence automatic and not necessarily within our
control.
This has repercussions in terms of language learning as
well. Our life does not so much depend upon making sense of the world since we
have already more or less successfully passed through that stage in our younger
years. However to finish on a more positive note, we can (and I would say
should), despite our brain and age, preserve a sense of wonder by occasionally
feeding the child within us and hence ensuring that this worldview or way of interpreting the world is still kept alive
and well.
2 comments:
What an illuminating piece this is! Thank you. Karleen and both have experience of premature babies, how they develop and get on later in life, so were particularly interested in that part. (They've both done well and ended up bigger than average in size: one taller, the other brawnier).
When you said smartphone, I first imagined you tapping away at its touchscreen keyboard, which I thought impossible, but Karleen pointed out that you could have taken an audio recording. Which is fair enough because you have captured a great deal of detail, perhaps the full gist of the lecture. Some achievement! I'll be coming back to this post.
Thank you, Vincent, for your comment and praise! In fact, you imagined it right, I was indeed tapping away on my touchscreen keyboard ; )
Years of training in school and lecture settings have made me more effective and efficient at taking notes by focusing on the gist of it. The smartphone is more useful and quicker for me than traditional pen and paper; my only worry is that it may be mistakenly construed as me not paying attention or, worse, texting during the lecture. But I was indeed all ears.
The reception after the lecture is usually my opportunity to ask questions and clarify doubts and, in many cases, even elaborate on the issues. I also asked Dr. Aslin about research on electronic devices and the effect of screen time on child development. Although this was not his domain, he gave me information on who to check out and potentially contact regarding that information.
That topic, alongside premature babies, is also close to my heart as my child, among many others I'm sure, has been engaging in that new medium. I believe that in the end, just like being premature, the brain and human body will find ways to turn it into an advantage. Once I have more concrete information, I shall post it here of course ; )
Post a Comment